<p>
[quote]
Secondly, to the, limited, extent this effect was found in science at all, it is clear that it has diminished over time (Willis and MacNamee, 1990)
[/quote]
</p>
<p>W&M only proves my point further. Sure, they may have found that the effect had diminished over time, but that it still exists. After all, who really cares about the effect over time? Sure, according to W&M, maybe the effects of the invisible college will have disappeared completely in the future. But so what? By that time, you're dead. Nobody has a time machine; you have to determine what is the best school for you right now. </p>
<p>
[quote]
First of all, much of this work found these effects outside of science,
[/quote]
</p>
<p>Then consider the studies that were found within the realm of science. The biased impact of prestige on scientific peer-review has been a problem basically ever since peer review was instituted. Let me give you some famous examples. </p>
<p>Consider what happened to the paper "An Experiment to show that a Divided Electric Current may be greater in both Branches than in the Mains", written by Lord Rayleigh - yes, THAT Lord Rayleigh who was one of the most highly regarded scientists of the late 1800's and who, among numerous discoveries, eventually won the Nobel Prize in Physics for his pioneering work on the physical properties of gases, including the discovery of argon. According to his son and biographer, Lord Rayleigh's name was accidentally omitted from the paper whereupon the Committee of the British Association promptly rejected it. "However, when the authorship was discovered, the paper was found to have merits after all." (Strutt 1924, Barber 1961)</p>
<p>Consider what happened to one of the mathematical papers of a young Neils Henrik Abel that made important advances of one of the classical problems on 5th degree equations. Abel would later become one of the most prominent mathematicians of his day, discovering such concepts as Abel's Theorem, Abel's transformation, and the abelian group, yet at that particular time not only was he unknown, but he came from a country (Norway, which at the time was part of the Kingdom of Denmark) that had little standing in the world of mathematics, most of the best mathematicians being located in France, one of the German principalities, the Netherlands, or the UK. Abel sent his paper to many of the leading mathematicians of the time, including the (at the time) world-famous Carl Gauss, but Gauss never even bothered to read it and simply filed it away unopened, where it was later found after Gauss' death. Of course, upon its rediscovery, the paper was found to be a highly important contribution to number theory. (Ore 1957) </p>
<p>But at least Rayleigh and Abel did achieve recognition during their time. Much sadder stories can be told about scientists whose works were marginalized due to lack of institutional prestige until long after they were dead. A classic example is the now-world-famous 1865 paper "Experiments on Plant Hybridization" by Gregor Mendel. We know of it now - in fact, we all learn of it in high school biology - but during his time, Mendel's work was widely discredited because he was not a faculty member of a prestigious university or, heck, any university at all, but was 'merely' a monk at an obscure abbey, and hence his work was widely discredited as being that of "an insignificant provincial" by such luminaries of biology/botany at the time as W.O. Focke, von Marilaun, and Hoffman (Roberts 1929). In fact, Mendel conducted a correspondence with the prominent and famous German Swiss botanist Karl von Nageli who was harshly critical to Mendel's ideas and even suggested that he change his experiments from peas (for which his genetic theories were unusually suitable) to hawkweed (which was highly unsuitable), which caused Mendel to became so discouraged that he eventually abandoned his genetic research altogether to become a full-time abbott( Beveridge 1959, Krumbiegel 1957). Mendel died in complete obscurity, and his groundbreaking paper was rediscovered only in 1900, 16 years after his death and 35 years after it was first presented.</p>
<p>Or consider the seminal work on continental drift by Alfred Wegener. Wegener first presented his ideas in 1912, but he suffered greatly from the problems that, first, he did not hold a degree in geology (his PhD was in astronomy/meteorology), and even worse, he did not hold a fully-fledged professorship (his position at the time was merely a 'tutor'). Hence, his ideas were so widely disbelieved that the American Association of Petroleum Geologists held a conference specifically to discredit them, and the reaction from the geology community was near-universal dismissal, so much so, that Wegener stopped publishing work on geology and went back to meteorological research, eventually dying on a meteorological expedition to Greenland. Only 20-30 years later was Wegener's papers rediscovered and found to have significant merit, so much so that his theories are now accepted as the current paradigm within modern geophysics. (Oreskes 2004) </p>
<p>Or consider the sad story of Nicolae Paulescu, who is now seen as the first true discoverer of insulin, as opposed to Frederick Banting and JJ MacLeod who won the Nobel Prize for their "discovery". Paulescu not only wrote 4 papers about his discovery, but also patented it, and Banting and MacLeod's Prize-winning paper merely confirmed Paulescu's findings and even cited them. Paulescu's work was nevertheless completely ignored by both the scientific community and the Nobel committee, and his pioneering work was not rediscovered until 50 years later. Paulescu's problem seemed to be that he worked at the University of Bucharest, an institution of little prestige relative to the premier standing of the University of Toronto that Banting and MacLeod were from, and that Paulescu was also known for holding anti-Semitic views, which is of course deplorable, but what does that have to do with the merits of your scientific findings? (Murray 1971)</p>
<p>Now, some of you may be thinking that these are mere anecdotes and do not demonstrate a true pattern. Au contraire- I just happened to pick some of the more sensational stories. In fact, biases have been revealed again and again throughout the scientific process. For example, in an article in Nature, Wenneras & Wold 1997 revealed significant gender and nepotism within the peer review process: a matched analysis revealed that women had to be 2.5 times more productive than men in order to receive the same competency scores, but that that ratio could be reduced if that woman had a personal affiliation with one of the reviewers. Sandstrom & Hallsten 2008 extended the findings of W&W and found that nepotism within the scientific grant review process was an even bigger problem than even W&W found. </p>
<p>
[quote]
Further, the cause of this observation is debatable, and many have maintained that it is due to common approaches to methods and interpretation, rather than simple favoritism toward members of the "club".
[/quote]
</p>
<p>And that's merely a distinction without a difference. For after all, if it really is only a matter of common approaches to methods and interpretation, then one has to ask how one learns these common approaches if they are not members of the club. Put another way, if you are a member of the 'club', then you will be provided more opportunities to learn these common approaches as opposed to somebody else who isn't a member of the club. </p>
<p>One feature of this sentiment was expressed in Nature by Herrera 1997, where he stated that part of being in 'the club' was being able to write your articles in fluent English which obviously discriminates against the vast majority of the world who does not speak English as a native language. Similarly, one should expect that if a dominant paradigm in terms of research methods are being used by certain highly prestigious universities that also happen to control the chokepoints of publication, then it behooves you to attend those universities so that you can learn those research methods and hence learn how to pass the chokepoints. </p>
<p>
[quote]
It is particularly difficult to propose such favoritism in blind reviewed journals. Yes, sometimes it is clear who has written the paper, but most of the time it is not.
[/quote]
</p>
<p>Uh, I would argue that it is particularly easy to propose such favoritism. Let's be honest. Many (probably most) fields of scientific study are comprised of quite small research communities where most of the researchers know most of the other researchers and, by and large, know who is doing what. For example, when Watson and Crick were researching the structure of DNA, they knew full well that they were racing to compete against Linus Pauling's team who was also attempting to answer the exact same question, and everybody knew that the stakes of the race were nothing short of a Nobel Prize. Look, scientists in any field all go to the same conferences where they present their findings, and then when you receive a paper to be reviewed that is 'exactly the same' as a conference presentation you attended, it doesn't exactly take a genius to figure out who the authors are. </p>
<p>
[quote]
If her boyfriend reviewed her paper he was profoundly unethical. If the editor found out about it the journal should have retracted the paper and included an editorial explaining how the former reviewer- never to be used again by that or any other reputable journal- had corrupted the process. This would be hugely humiliating for all concerned. If the author had known her lover was refereeing her paper and went along with it then she was profoundly unsuited for an academic career. Certainly it would rule her out for being hired or promote
[/quote]
</p>
<p>Maybe it is unethical. But to echo W&W and S&H, whether we like it or not, the effect of nepotism within the sciences is widespread. </p>
<p>Look, like I said before, the scientific community for most research topics is pretty small such that you often times can't help having some sort of personal relationship (either friendly or unfriendly) with at least one of your reviewers for any of your papers. Like it or not, that's how it is. This is why so many researchers decry the political nature of academia. </p>
<p>The bottom line is this. Let's not kid ourselves; peer review and the scientific promotion system are far from immaculate. It would be nice to believe that, in the world of science, all that matters is the quality of your research. But that's not reality. These systems have numerous and significant sociological biases such as the bias for prestige, as I'm sure Mendel, Wegener, and Paulescu would agree. In fact it would be tremendously surprising if they didn't. After all, they are manmade constructs and are hence subject to the same sociological forces as any other manmade constructs (Kuhn 1962). Since this is the system that we have, the best thing for you do to is to have the biases work in your favor, not against you.</p>