<p>Some historical context…</p>
<p>I may have the name and title slightly off because I read it a LONG time ago. There is a book by someone named Boyer. I think the first name is Allan, but that may not be the correct spelling and I’m too lazy to google right now. He wrote a book for the Carnegie Foundation–at least that’s my recollection–which is entitled something like “College in America.” </p>
<p>Reading that book will explain how we got where we are. You see, someone actually did a study to figure out which kids succeeded in college, expecially top colleges. Believe it or not, ECs DO have predictive value. The study compared students with few or no ECs with students who had lots of them or who were passionate about one or two.
If you control for gpa and test scores, the kids with substantial high school ECs graduated from college at a higher rate and had higher GPAs than those without such ECs.</p>
<p>And, so, once the study came out, selective colleges started to take ECs into account. </p>
<p>It’s a really old book, but it does explain how ECs became more important. </p>
<p>Now, I think this approach missed something. When nobody knew that ECs could be all that important for admissions purposes, the result of the study was kind of a no brainer. If two kids have 3.8 unweighted gpas at the same high school and both have identifical test scores, then it’s likely that the kid with substantial ECs will succeed. But personally, I think that’s in part because the kind of kids who did ECs back in the 1950s and early 1960s did them because they wanted to and that sort of initiative did demonstrate a certain quality that made it more likely these kids would do well. Plus, if kids were spending 15 hours a week on ECs and doing well, when they encountered a more rigourous work load in college, they could drop the ECs or limit them and still do well. They had an additonal 10-15 hours a week. A certain percentage of kids with no ECs were working 24/7 to get those grades and test scores. When they encountered more difficult college work, they couldn’t spend any more time on school work than they had in high school and some weren’t able to keep up with increased demands. </p>
<p>In any event, that study, which was sponsored by the Carnegie Foundation lead to a change in admissions practices. </p>
<p>The problem is that now there are a lot of kids who participate in ECs in high school simply to increase their admissions odds. While the time constraints are still relevant, they simply aren’t the same sort of kids who did ECs in the late 50s and early 60s for reasons which had nothing whatsoever to do with getting into college. </p>
<p>In any event, LadyDi is simply wrong when she says there is no correlation between ECs and academic success in college. There is if you control for gpa and test scores. So, it is perfectly valid, IMO, for a college like Amherst to take ECs into consideration when choosing among academically qualified candidates–though I suspect the correlation now wouldn’t be as strong as it was before everyone got the memo to do ECs.</p>
<p>I dislike the Oxbridge interview method because it plays to extroverts even more than the US one. Oxbridge has studied its process too and one of the conclusions was that when upper middle class people, especially males for some reason, interview secondary students, they tend to weed out kids from the working classes. These kids have different accents that sound uneducated. It affects perceptions. Kids form the right “public” schools were coached as to what to wear at an interview. Kids from grammar schools–or whatever the correct term is now–would show up wearing what some interviewers deemed inappropriate clothing and they were subconsciously marked down for it. </p>
<p>Plus, kids from public school had been drilled in interview techniques. They may never have answered the same question but they went through mock interviews. Kids from government funded schools were far less likely to receive such coaching.</p>