<p>It's a pretty old topic as far as what's in vogue in linguistics is concerned, but I rediscovered this Language Log article today -- Language</a> Log: The SAT fails a grammar test. (Language Log, by the way, is a blog written by a panel of linguists acclaimed in their field, from Penn's Liberman to Stanford's Zwicky ...) </p>
<p>Anyway, the common stereotype of the perfect 2400 student as "just a good test taker" may not be perfectly unjustified, and it comes to mind that those who are linguistically-trained or linguistically-inclined would do worse in these sort of sections. Students who actually apply grammatical and linguistic concept rather than memorising lists of rules (which is by the way, <em>NOT</em> what grammar is truly about, as any linguistics student trained in the field of universal grammar knows...) might ironically be prone to do worse on the language sections of the test than those who do not have an affinity for linguistics -- the type of student to simply think grammar as a list of rules of language some authority made up. (As opposed to looking at the evolutionary reasons of why grammar is present in all fully-fledged natural human languages.)</p>
<p>In the language portions of the SAT, the student not only has to use what he or she knows, but what he or she knows about what the test-makers do not know.</p>
<p>
</p>
<p>I am antagonising about this mainly for future generations -- are the people who set the test questions on language even linguistically-trained?</p>
<p>Linguists can even go so far as to argue that the SAT "curriculum" partially contributes to the linguistic ignorance of the public -- an ignorance that later on makes them susceptible to dangerous political rhetoric that justifies badly-formed public policy as it relates to language, especially where issues of linguistic diversity and the whole mess of "English as a national language" are concerned. Grammar is more than a set of arbitrary rules -- but a set of rules that have arisen for biological and psychological reasons, reasons that are actually rather intricately tied to advanced math and information theory. But the way the SAT is set up, people are led to believe, "oh I got 700 because I don't know <em>all</em> the rules of grammar, and that's okay because the rules of grammar are so vast anyway and only a few people know all of them." </p>
<p>WHICH is not the case -- native speakers subconsciously and alway know all the grammatical rules of their native language -- those who are mentally-handicapped (e.g. patients who have suffered damage to Broca's area) excepted. However, not everyone knows how to consciously <em>analyse</em> grammar, and when digesting (or writing) fairly abstruse texts that exceed the "working memory" of grammar, the innate, instinctive capacity that tells us how to form grammatical sentences often falters. That's where conscious analysis is useful, and that's why it's also useful to test one's ability to make such analysis. But the "prescriptivist culture" of the SAT would have us believe otherwise.</p>